You must turn off your ad blocker to use Psych Web; however, we are taking pains to keep advertising minimal and unobtrusive (one ad at the top of each page) so interference to your reading should be minimal.




If you need instructions for turning off common ad-blocking programs, click here.

If you already know how to turn off your ad blocker, just hit the refresh icon or F5 after you do it, to see the page.

Psi man mascot

Mindfulness

In the 1980s, psychology started its own meditation movement. Jon Kabat-Zinn, who was introduced to meditation by Zen advocate Philip Kapleau, starting using mindfulness meditation as a therapy for chronic pain patients.

The term "mindfulness" is one of several translations of the Buddhist concept of sati. In Buddhism this meant monitoring memories and experience to prevent worldly concerns such as desire and craving.

In 1989, Susan Langer of Harvard wrote a book called Mindfulness, introducing more psychologists to the topic. In 1994, Kabat-Zinn's second book, Wherever You Go, There You Are became a national bestseller in the U.S., and interest in mindfulness accelerated.

Google's ngram service, which provides objective data about how often a word or phrase occurs in books scanned by Google, showed what happened to the word "mindfulness."

Graph shows references to mindfulness rapidly accelerating
Google ngram data shows how frequently the word "mindfulness" was used in literature

As you can see, the word was used a few times in the 1950s and 1960s. But then the concept of mindfulness really took off around 1980 and climbed in usage ever since.

As mindfulness meditation was made the subject of thousands of psychological studies, the concept strayed from its Buddhist origins. As of 2017, a search online to answer the question, "What is mindful­ness?" produced answers like this:

That last definition comes from Kabat-Zinn, who introduced the term to psychologists. In his 2005 book, Kabat-Zinn explained that part of his inspiration came from taking care of his father, an eminent immunologist, who died of Alzheimer's Disease.

Kabat-Zinn watched his father lose all sense of who he was and what was happen­ing to him. It re-emphasized the importance of giving full apprec­iation to the present moment and making the best use of awareness while we have it.

In 1982, Kabat-Zinn published an article reporting on an outpatient program for chronic pain based on mindfulness meditation. While only preliminary results were reported, they were encouraging.

In 1985, Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, and Burney (1985) presented more complete results. They had trained 90 chronic pain patients in mindfulness meditation during a 10-week Stress Reduction and Relaxation Program.

What did Kabat-Zinn report?

Afterwards the clients showed many improvements. These included "statistically significant reductions...in measures of present-moment pain, negative body image, inhibition of activity by pain, symptoms, mood disturbance, and psychological symptomatology, including anxiety and depression."

That was the beginning of three decades of research using mind­fulness as an independent variable. The list of benefits reads like an infomercial. Published studies showed that mindfulness could...

Scattered in with the overwhelmingly positive results were a few negative reports. For example, mindfulness failed to help with smoking cessation.

Many of the positive changes listed above were measured using ques­tionnaires administered after mindfulness training. That form of measurement could easily be influenced by positive expectations.

A medication claiming benefits like those above would meet with requests for well-controlled double-blind studies. That is difficult with mindfulness.

How do you do a double-blind test of mindfulness? A convincing placebo therapy would have to be devised, and the person administer­ing it would have to be convinced of its efficacy just as a typical mindfulness instructor would be about mindfulness. This could be done, but not easily.

Meta-analysis of Mindfulness Effects

Recognizing many methodological problems in mindfulness research (which they summarized) Goyal et al. (2013) decided on a critical, systematic review and meta-analysis of all mindfulness research they could locate. The group consisted of 16 researchers, all from Johns Hopkins University.

Goyal et al. surveyed 18,753 published studies of mindfulness, looking for randomized clinical trials with active controls. 47 studies met their criterion. That is a quarter of 1% of the total. The other 99.75% of mindfulness studies were not well controlled.

After restricting their attention to the well-controlled studies of mindfulness, the research team summarized their findings this way:

Mindfulness meditation programs had moderate evidence of improved anxiety... depression... and pain... and low evidence of improved stress/distress and mental health-related quality of life.

We found...no effect or insufficient evidence of any effect of meditation programs on positive mood, attention, substance use, eating habits, sleep, and weight.

We found no evidence that meditation programs were better than any active treatment (i.e., drugs, exercise, and other behavioral therapies). (Goyal et al., 2013)

Is there something special about the particular approach Americans developed toward mindfulness meditation? Probably not. Different forms of meditation, not labeled as mindfulness and often associated with particular religious or philosophical traditions, claim very similar benefits:

As mindfulness research entered its fourth decade, attention shifted from demonstrating significant effects to exploring mechanisms. Research­ers looked for neural mechanisms of attentional control that might be influenced by meditation.

Cognitive scientists say attention requires alerting, orienting, and executive control. Circuits performing those actions must be quieted during meditation. That might involve wide­spread areas of the brain.

Alerting, orienting, and executive control, taken together, require brain areas in the front (prefrontal cortex), sides (temporal lobes), middle (posterior cingulate cortex and parietal lobes), the back of the brain (cerebellum), plus areas deep into the brainstem used for orienting and arousal. Brefczynski-Lewis, Lutz, Schaefer, Levinson, and Davidson (2007) showed that expert meditators quieted this "network of brain regions typically involved in sustained attention."

In addition to avoiding sustained attention, meditators must quiet more introspective processes such as daydreaming or wondering about other people's mental states. Those activities are associated with the Default Mode Network (DMN) of the brain.

As expected, mindfulness meditation was found to be associated with reduced activity in the DMN (Taylor et al., 2013). Those researchers suggested that reduced Default Mode Network activity was consistent with "strengthened present-moment awareness."

---------------------
References:

Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007) Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 104, 11483-11488. doi:10.1073/pnas.0606552104

Goyal, M. et al. [15 more authors] Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 357-368. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33-47.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life (10th anniversary ed.). New York: Hyperion.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to Our Senses: Healing Ourselves and the World through Mindfulness. New York: Hyperion.

Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., & Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8, 163-190.

Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.

Malinowski, P. (2013, February 04) Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Neuroscience. Retrieved from: https://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnins.2013.00008/full https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00008

Taylor, V. A., Daneault, V., Grant, J., Scavone, G., Breton, E. Roffe-Vidal, S., Courtemanche, J., Lavarenne, A.S., Marrelec, G., Benali, H., & Beauregard, M. (2013) Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 4-14. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr087


Write to Dr. Dewey at psywww@gmail.com.


Don't see what you need? Psych Web has over 1,000 pages, so it may be elsewhere on the site. Do a site-specific Google search using the box below.